Skip to main content

Climate change and climate variability: personal motivation for adaptation and mitigation

Abstract

Background

Global climate change impacts on human and natural systems are predicted to be severe, far reaching, and to affect the most physically and economically vulnerable disproportionately. Society can respond to these threats through two strategies: mitigation and adaptation. Industry, commerce, and government play indispensable roles in these actions but so do individuals, if they are receptive to behavior change. We explored whether the health frame can be used as a context to motivate behavioral reductions of greenhouse gas emissions and adaptation measures.

Methods

In 2008, we conducted a cross-sectional survey in the United States using random digit dialing. Personal relevance of climate change from health threats was explored with the Health Belief Model (HBM) as a conceptual frame and analyzed through logistic regressions and path analysis.

Results

Of 771 individuals surveyed, 81% (n = 622) acknowledged that climate change was occurring, and were aware of the associated ecologic and human health risks. Respondents reported reduced energy consumption if they believed climate change could affect their way of life (perceived susceptibility), Odds Ratio (OR) = 2.4 (95% Confidence Interval (CI): 1.4 - 4.0), endanger their life (perceived severity), OR = 1.9 (95% CI: 1.1 - 3.1), or saw serious barriers to protecting themselves from climate change, OR = 2.1 (95% CI: 1.2 - 3.5). Perceived susceptibility had the strongest effect on reduced energy consumption, either directly or indirectly via perceived severity. Those that reported having the necessary information to prepare for climate change impacts were more likely to have an emergency kit OR = 2.1 (95% CI: 1.4 - 3.1) or plan, OR = 2.2 (95% CI: 1.5 -3.2) for their household, but also saw serious barriers to protecting themselves from climate change or climate variability, either by having an emergency kit OR = 1.6 (95% CI: 1.1 - 2.4) or an emergency plan OR = 1.5 (95%CI: 1.0 - 2.2).

Conclusions

Motivation for voluntary mitigation is mostly dependent on perceived susceptibility to threats and severity of climate change or climate variability impacts, whereas adaptation is largely dependent on the availability of information relevant to climate change. Thus, the climate change discourse could be framed from a health perspective to motivate behaviour change.

Peer Review reports

Background

Humans are now unequivocally implicated in contributing to global climate change [1, 2]. Strategic action is required both from individuals and the private/public sector to prevent harmful corollaries from climate change to individuals and society at large. Climate change will alter the probability of extreme weather events, which have been associated with adverse health outcomes, such as heat-related mortality and morbidity during heat waves [3, 4]; injuries from extreme weather events [5]; injuries and death from flooding [6]; re- and emerging communicable diseases such as hantavirus associated hemorrhagic fever, West Nile fever, or Lyme disease [7]. Dramatic episodes, such as the European heat wave of 2003 or hurricane Katrina in 2005 can be seen in isolation or as part of a probability function of events with increasing frequency, duration, and intensity. Thus, the difficulty in building understanding of climate change lies in the fact that many climate change-related events such as natural disasters or disease outbreaks cannot be directly attributed to climate change making it less intuitive and thus difficult to communicate.

In order to gauge the motivation of individuals to take precautionary steps to reduce climate risks we explored options for mitigation and adaptation. Mitigation entails reductions in greenhouse gas emissions and augmentation in greenhouse gas sinks intended to minimize the extent of global warming [8]. These steps include energy conservation by increasing the fuel efficiency of vehicles; switching to cleaner energy sources by changing business practices; or carbon sequestration through tropical reforestation. However, these practices have proven to be remarkably slow and difficult to implement at best. Even if so, their impact on global climate change will not be noticed in decades to come due to the longevity of greenhouse gases in the atmosphere [2]. Thus, adaptation to climate change impacts becomes a necessity both on an individual and communal level [9, 10]. Adaptation entails adjustments of environmental or social settings in response to past, current or anticipated climatic events and their impacts in order to moderate their consequences [2]. Autonomous (or spontaneous) adaptation is typically defined as responding to climate-driven changes in natural systems that occur naturally by private actors without intervention of public institutions [8]. It is usually the result of reactive responses to current climate impacts, rather than preventive measures. In contrast, anticipatory (proactive or planned) adaptation is initiated prior to climate change impacts are observed. It is based on scientific information about projected impacts and is usually executed by government agencies [2, 11].

How then can the public be motivated to take mitigation and adaptation steps?

Climate change has traditionally been framed as an environmental, rather than a health issue. Concerns of ecologic, environmental, social, or economic climate change impacts are certainly important drivers of behaviour change but may have contributed to the recent "climate fatigue" [12]. Meanwhile, it is known that personal perception of risk is the strongest motivator of health behaviour change based on the health promotion literature [13]. Potentially then, the health aspects of climate change should resonate well across wide segments of the American public [14]. Thus, health could be a strong motivating factor for individuals to reduce greenhouse gas emissions and to adopt adaptation measures to reduce health risks. However, it is not clear if the health frame would suffice to engage the pubic in adaptation and mitigation steps, since they hinge on public appreciation of the health threats of these climatic processes. Besides, media coverage has been rather polarized and not constructively educated the public about potential health threats [15]. Very little research has been published to date on public perception of adverse health effects from climate change but some studies that have touched on this issue have not found the public to be very knowledgeable [16–18].

The goal of this study was to assess the health context as a motivating factor for adaptation and mitigation behavior. We had previously tested the transtheoretical model (stages of change model) to explain barriers to behaviour change for mitigation [19]. We also considered theory of reasoned action and social learning theory to elucidate underlying drivers of behaviour change [20, 21]. In order to test the hypothesis whether health is an appropriate frame for behaviour change in response to climate change risks we applied the health belief model (HBM) to gauge respondents' willingness to engage in voluntary mitigation and adaptation efforts based on their attitudes and beliefs [22]. The HBM had originally been developed to explain the likelihood of health-related behavior from an individual perspective. The HBM has been widely used to understand preventive health behaviors as well as mitigation behavior to reduce environmental pollution that has human health impacts [23–25]. The components of the HBM are perception of susceptibility, severity, benefits, and barriers to action, as well as cues to action and self-efficacy. Perceived susceptibility and perceived barriers have been shown to be associated with preventive behavior while perceived severity, perceived benefits, and perceived barriers have been most strongly associated with treatment of a condition.

We hypothesized that if climate change is perceived as a health threat, then the components of the HBM might be able to predict mitigation and adaptation behavior. We probed whether respondents considered themselves to be susceptible to the threats of climate change/variability and whether this health risk was deemed severe. We explored whether or not the HBM could explain respondents' propensity for autonomous adaptation behavior, and whether or not they had engaged in voluntary energy reduction to counteract global climate change (mitigation behavior). The time-frame for mitigation is long-range while adaptation is more immediate but both actions are important and complementary, and are not mutually exclusive [26]. Thus, these findings are important if public health agencies are to reach the public with behavior change messages through social marketing or communication campaigns using the health context as a frame.

Methods

Survey Methods

Attitudes about climate change/variability impacts, mitigation and adaptation were assessed through random-digit dial telephone surveys from a U.S. sample between September and October, 2008 (table 1). The surveys were conducted in both Spanish and English at Portland State University's Survey Research Laboratory (SRL) which has been described elsewhere [19]. The SRL is outfitted with a state-of the-art Computer Assisted Telephone Interviewing (CATI) system with 20 phone interviewing booths. The survey questions appeared on a monitor and were read by the interviewer in a preset order. The survey was designed with complex contingency patterns of questions, where sub-questions were automatically branched off to produce skip patterns. Invalid responses were recognized by the CATI system which enhanced data quality. Furthermore, the need for subsequent data entry was omitted since the data were typed directly into the database. Quality control was assured by a centralized facility that monitored the interviews. The original sample of phone numbers was selected based on the census distribution of population density across all U.S. states in order to assure a geographically representative study population. The optimum time for calling was established through call-back procedures (three call-backs per number) and interview scheduling. On average an interview took 17 minutes and the survey was administered over the course of 33 days. Participants were screened for age (> 18 years), comprehension, zip codes (to assure geographic specificity of respondents) and knowledge of global climate change. Respondents denying climate change as a phenomenon were censured (15%), because all questions pertained to different aspects of climate change [16]. The survey instrument and study protocol was approved by PSU's Human Subjects Research Review Committee (HSRRC Proposal #04157).

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of study population, United States, 2008

The ranking of respondents per U.S. state in our study population correlated highly with the ranking of the U.S. census population density by state (Spearman correlation: r = 0.897; p < 0.001); thus the study sample was reasonably representative of the geographic distribution of the U.S. population. The demographic profile of the sample (table 1) was also reasonably representative of the US population at large (according to population data from the 2000 census) with respect to gender (Census female 50.9%, analysis sample 56.8%) and race (Census white 75.1%, analysis sample 81.4%), although there was a certain sampling bias towards the age, educational qualifications and household income, since our sample reflected the populations that were more likely to be home during the day, a common occurrence in phone surveys [27–29].

In order to further explain and predict health behaviors related to climate change/variability we drew on the HBM, a psychological model that focuses on attitudes and beliefs of individuals [30]. Survey questions were designed to capture predictors of mitigation and autonomous adaptation behavior through the lens of the HBM components: perception of susceptibility, severity, benefits, and barriers to action, as well as cues to action and self-efficacy. Categorical demographic variables were dichotomized due to sample size constraints (table 1). Responses to open ended questions were subjected to content analysis.

Statistical modelling

We first estimated three parallel logistic regression equations within a single model in order to obtain estimates of the association between all the predictors (demographics and HMB related variables) with the three outcomes (mitigation: reduced energy consumption; autonomous adaptation: emergency kit and emergency plan). The causal structure implied by the HBM was not imposed on the data, but the correlation between the three dependent variables was taken into account which were jointly modeled. We employed the robust maximum likelihood (MLR) estimator with Gauss-Hermite integration. In the second stage of the analysis we employed a path analytic approach to estimate the parameters representing the causal structure which is implied by the HBM (see figure 1). For example the effect of perceived susceptibility to the three outcomes is mediated by perceived severity and therefore an indirect effect is implied. Path analysis allows the formal estimation of indirect effects and their associated standard errors, which is not possible with the standard regression approach we used in the first analytic stage, since with this the effect of all the variables in the model is simply adjusted for the presence of all the other covariates [31].

Figure 1
figure 1

Conceptual path diagram of the Health Belief Model.

In the presence of binary or ordinal mediators or endogenous variables (perceived severity for example) it has been suggested that the product of standardized probit coefficients is the most reliable estimate of the indirect effect [32]. The probit model assumes that a latent continuum underlies the observed binary or ordinal variables. The standardised probit regression coefficients can therefore be interpreted as the probability of a one standard deviation change in the underlying continuous variable attributable to a one standard deviation change in the predictor. The path analysis was estimated with the Weighted Least Squares, mean and variance adjusted (WLSMV) estimator, in the Mplus 5.21 software [33]. Due to the complexity of the model and the nature of our sample we report significance tests derived by bootstrapped standard errors with 10,000 replications.

Results

Of the 771 individuals surveyed 3% (n = 23) had never heard of climate change before and 15% (n = 112) did not believe climate change was in fact occurring. However, the majority of Americans (n = 622; 81%) in our survey were aware of climate change and believed that it was certainly taking place. Study participants, when prompted, attributed a number of environmental phenomena to climate change, most noticeably average temperature increases and the melting of permafrost in the Arctic region (table 2). Heat waves, more frequent storms, water shortages, sea-level rise, flooding, and loss of wildlife habitat were mentioned by four out of five respondents that knew about climate change. Misconceptions about climate change impacts were revealed in 10% of comments provided to open-ended questions (n = 135), such as earthquakes, depletion of the ozone layer, vitamin depletion in food, etc. Perceived health risks to the American public are listed in table 3. Respondents were concerned about both air and water quality impacts and about heat stroke and respiratory problems. The most common health concern in the open ended comments related to food shortages (21% of n = 126).

Table 2 Perceived environmental, ecological or societal impacts from climate change in the United States, 2008
Table 3 Perceived health risk from climate change, United States, 2008

Perceived susceptibility was further explored by asking respondents if climate change could affect their way of life or lifestyle; 78% of respondents recognized a certain level of susceptibility (table 4). Of the respondents, 69% reported that climate change could potentially endanger their lives and pose adverse personal effects. However, 62% were under the impression that personal preparation could save their life. Only a minority of study participants (31%) saw any obstacles or barriers to protecting themselves from negative consequences of climate change. They included lack of money or resources (65%), lack of help from others (56%), lack of knowledge (53%), lack of personal energy or motivation (43%), or lack of time (34%) (table 5). The majority (56%) of respondents felt that they both had the necessary information to prepare for climate change impacts as well as the confidence and ability to protect themselves from health impacts of climate change. Based on these findings we explored whether the respondents were ready for behavioural change and to take mitigation or adaptation actions.

Table 4 Survey questions of climate change mitigation and adaptation, United States 2008
Table 5 Self-reported obstacles to protect oneself from climate change impacts, United States, 2008

The majority of respondents (77%) reported having reduced their energy consumption based on what they have heard about global climate change. Reported energy conservation steps (N = 479) are listed in table 6. Virtually everybody claimed to have reduced their home energy consumption and their gasoline use. Energy intensive commodities such as water and food were also considered by four out of five respondents. Among those that did not report any energy conservation efforts (18%; n = 118) cited inconvenience and lack of conviction as reasons (table 7).

Table 6 Self-reported steps in energy conservation, United Stated, 2008
Table 7 Self-reported obstacles to energy conservation, United States, 2008

The vast majority of study participants affirmed autonomous adaptive behavior during an extremely hot weather period. These steps included cooling off in a room with air-conditioning (89%) or with a fan (79%), staying out of the sun (96%), drinking plenty of water (99%), and dressing lightly (88%), reduced exercise (60%). For other types of climatic events, 52% individuals reported having an emergency kit that included such items as a first aid kit, thermometers, flashlight and batteries, food that won't spoil, sufficient drinking water, and other essential items in the event of a disaster or emergency. Among those that reported not having one (n = 298) 23% admitted never having thought about it and 3% did not expect to need one; procrastination and laziness were some of the other reasons for not having one. Of those that provided open ended comments (n = 82) 35% reported having some of the items but not all and were planning to reassemble them into a kit in the near future. Qualitative comments described some of the circumstances: "Usually we get tornados, and when there is a tornado there is no time to get the kit when the emergency is happening." and "We are not in a flood plain and once in a while we have had a tornado, but it is so rare. I keep some of the stuff on the shelf but not in a specific kit." Respondents were also asked whether their household currently had a plan for what to do to protect themselves and their family in the event of a disaster or emergency, such as how to evacuate the home, or how to stay in contact with other family members; 57% claimed having such a plan. Of those that did not have an emergency plan (n = 115) 59% never considered it and 5% did not get around to it.

With respect to the predictive power of the HBM constructs as independent variables, in table 8 we present the odd ratios from the three parallel logistic regression models. We observed a positive association between perceived severity and mitigation (OR = 1.874, p < 0.001), perceived susceptibility and mitigation (OR = 2.364, p < 0.001), as well as perceived barriers and mitigation (OR = 2.052, p < 0.001). Furthermore, gender was associated with mitigation (OR = 1.885, p < 0.001), with women being 1.88 times more likely to take voluntary mitigation actions. Having an emergency kit was positively associated with perceived barriers (OR = 1.608, p < 0.001) and cues to action (OR = 2.098, p < 0.001), whereas perceived susceptibility (OR = 1.614, p < 0.001), perceived barriers (OR = 1.476, p < 0.05) and cues to action (OR = 2.161, p < 0.001), were all positively associated with having an emergency plan. Finally gender was negatively associated with having an emergency kit (OR = 0.577, p < 0.001) and an emergency plan (OR = 0.883, p < 0.001), with women being less likely to engage in any of the two autonomous adaptation actions.

Table 8 Odds Ratios and 95% Confidence Intervals from the three simultaneous logistic regression models, United States, 2008

In table 9 we present the standardized probit regression coefficients derived from the path analytic model, in order to estimate the influence of each pathway implied by the HBM. We did not observe any significant effect of the demographic characteristics and SEP status of the participants in the endogenous variables -mediators (perceived threat, perceived benefits and perceived barriers) of the HBM. Consequently none of the indirect effects of the demographic and SEP indicators on the three outcomes was significant. On the contrary we observed a strong positive association between perceived severity and mitigation, β = 0.479, p < 0.001. Similarly we observed a strong indirect effect of perceived susceptibility on mitigation via perceived severity, β = 0.349, p < 0.001, which was dominated by the very strong association between perceived susceptibility and perceived severity, β = 0.728, p < 0.001. Perceived benefits had a positive association with mitigation, β = 0.204, p < 0.001, as did perceived barriers, β = 0.322, p < 0.001. We did not observe any other significant association between the HBM variables and mitigation.

Table 9 Standardised probit regression parameters, decomposed to direct and indirect effects, United States, 2008

With respect to the two autonomous adaptation actions, we observed a positive association between perceived susceptibility and having an emergency kit, β = 0.100, p < 0.01, as well as an indirect effect of perceived susceptibility via perceived severity, β = 0.108, p < 0.01. Perceived benefits had also a significant association with having an emergency kit, β = 0.108, p < 0.01, as did perceived barriers, β = 0.213, p < 0.001. We did not observe any other significant association between the HBM variables and having an emergency kit. Finally, perceived barriers had a positive association with having an emergency plan, β = 0.160, p < 0.001. We did not observe any other significant associations between the HBM variables and having a plan. Based on the results of both analytic approaches (parallel logistic regressions and path analysis) we estimated a refined path analytic model adding direct effects from cues to action and gender to the three outcomes. The added results are presented as underlined parameters in table 9. As expected from the logistic regression results cues to action were positively associated with having an emergency kit and an emergency plan, whereas gender was positively associated with mitigation and negatively with both autonomous adaptation actions.

Discussion

In the present study we explored perception of climate change risks among those who were not dismissive of climate change [16]. We tested the predictive power of the HBM with respect to respondents' propensity for autonomous adaptation behavior and mitigation behavior. We employed two statistical modeling approaches in order to test this: i) three parallel logistic regressions to obtain fully adjusted parameters for all the HBM constructs as well as demographics and socio-economic predictors; ii) a path analysis, where the causal structure implied by the HBM (see figure 1) was taken into account in the estimation of the model. This allowed us to estimate the indirect effects of perceived susceptibility, cues to action and demographic characteristics - all via perceived severity - on the three outcomes.

Our findings on environmental impacts from climate change are very similar to recent surveys conducted in the U.S., Canada and Malta with 60-80% of respondents anticipating environmental threats [16]. Climate change or climate variability is perceived as posing a risk which sets the stage for behaviour change. Almost 8 in 10 respondents who had heard about global climate change reported having reduced their energy consumption; this self-reported mitigation effort was associated with a sense of susceptibility as well as the severity of climate change, with the effect of susceptibility being both direct as well as mediated by severity, thus making it the strongest predictor of mitigation effort. Furthermore, while respondents felt that there were certain barriers to protecting themselves from the negative consequences of climate change they also felt that their mitigation actions had co-benefits, such as reduced energy bills.

Intentional reduction in energy consumption by individuals hinges on their state of awareness and concern about climate change, their willingness to act and their ability to change [34, 35]. Thus, it is important to portray voluntary mitigation as necessary and achievable [36]. Low impact energy conservation was acted upon but not on high energy savings: curtailment (home energy conservation such as switching off lights; driving less; etc) was readily embraced (or at least reported) in contrast to efficiency improvements (switching to a fuel-efficient car or appliance) which was not, despite the fact, that later would deliver higher energy savings. Other high energy savings activities such as flying less or walking more were comparatively underreported in our survey which has also been reported elsewhere [37]. It is important to note that respondents were prompted about these different activities and might as a result have overstated their true motivation for these actions [38]. While interviewers specifically asked about climate change-related behavior change other factors (such as utility bills and gasoline prices) could also have contributed to behavior change. In addition, we did not attempt to quantify the extent of energy reduction based on these self-reported mitigation activities. These data should be considered an indication that the respondents would be willing to tackle mitigation steps but not necessarily being actively engaged in climate change mitigation.

The majority of respondents also reported having taken steps towards autonomous adaptation to extreme weather events attributable to climate variability or climate change. Study participants that were aware of climate change attributed a number of environmental impacts to climate change such as average temperature increase, heat waves, drought conditions or water shortage. Heat stroke or heat exhaustion, stress or anxieties were listed as major health concerns from these environmental impacts. In the context of the climate change interview, over half of respondents (52%) asserted having prepared an emergency kit with essential items needed in the event of a disaster or emergency and 57% claimed having a household emergency plan to protect themselves and their family in the event of a disaster or emergency. Both of these autonomous adaptation actions were positively influenced by cues to action, a finding which indicates that respondents felt they had the necessary information to prepare for the impacts of climate change. Similarly, both autonomous adaptation actions were influenced by perceived barriers, indicating that without removing those barriers the necessary information (cues to action) may not result in the desired behavioural change which is needed for successful adaptation. The majority of these barriers could be overcome with financial or practical support. There are a number of other autonomous adaptation actions or reactive responses to current climate impacts not covered in our survey. They could be supported with tax incentives and technical solutions and government agencies should work with communities to address their needs. From the other predictors only gender was associated with either autonomous adaptation action, with women appearing to be less adaptive. The majority of study participants had adapted to extreme weather conditions by cooling off in air conditioned places, reducing physical exertion or using a fan. These autonomous adaptation actions are reactive in nature, more so than proactive and thus does not capture anticipatory (proactive or planned) adaptive intentions. The survey instrument was initially designed to capture anticipatory adaptation as well by asking respondents if they adapted their home to climate change. For example: have you installed an A/C, insulation, insect screens, eliminated mosquitoes breeding sites, etc. However, due to low frequency responses these questions were eliminated from the analysis; thus the results apply to autonomous adaptation only and not to long-term impacts. Other studies that did not specifically examine the psychological constructs of the HBM have shown that adaptive behaviour to climate change may be more strongly linked to factors such as, environmental attitudes, political affiliation and attitudes towards scientists [39–41]. The relative differences in predictors of our mitigation and adaptation outcomes might in part be due to the wording of the survey questions. Nevertheless, health as a communication frame can be used to complement other strategies to augment the public response [42].

Attitudes and public perception of global climate change has also been examined in other surveys in the U.S. [19, 23, 40, 41, 43–47]. A recent study of US local public health department directors found that health directors are not actively responding to climate change in part due to their belief that the public does not have knowledge about the impact of climate change and therefore would be unwilling to support mitigation and adaptation activities [48]. Our study indicates that the majority of the public report awareness of environmental and health risks associated with climate change and that they consider themselves to be susceptible to being affected by it. Individuals with concerns about climate change hazards have been shown to be more engaged in personal actions [49, 50]. Our survey examined more specifically vulnerability and risk perception of climate change/vulnerability and indicates that the majority of the respondents would like to be part of climate solutions, which is consistent with other studies [51–54].

Conclusions

These findings indicate that the motivation for voluntary mitigation is mostly dependent on the perceived susceptibility to and severity of climate change, and autonomous adaptation is largely dependent on the availability of information relevant to climate change and its impact. Furthermore, our findings suggest an extension of the classic HBM as a predictive causal structure of mitigation and autonomous adaptation strategies, by adding direct effects of cues to action and gender to the these outcomes. Media advocacy campaigns should embrace the health context as a frame and aim at increasing general understanding of climate change and encourage active participation in mitigation and adaptation. Our findings indicate that proximal climate threats against which individuals feel highly susceptible, such as heat waves, droughts, or forest fires, are acted upon, especially when having the necessary information and if the threat is perceived as endangering their way of life. However, climate change is a multiplier of existing vulnerabilities for susceptible populations, underrepresented in our survey, who are at increased risk from such events. Vulnerable populations of low socio-economic status tend not to respond equally well to health promotion campaigns compared to the general population [55]. Thus, traditional media messages might not be able to persuade these populations to change behaviour and concerted efforts need to be put in place to reach these individuals both through more effective communication frames and community organizing [10, 56]. This study indicates how climate change can be framed from a health perspective to advance population health.

Abbreviations

OR:

Odds Ratio

CI:

Confidence Interval

HBM:

Health Belief Model

SRL:

Survey Research Laboratory

CATI:

Computer Assisted Telephone Interviewing

HSRRC:

Human Subjects Research Review Committee

A/C:

Air Conditioning.

References

  1. UNFCCC: Framework Convention on Climate Change. Article 1: Definitions.

  2. IPCC: Fourth Assessment Report, Synthesis Report. Fourth Assessment Report, Synthesis Report. 2007

    Google Scholar 

  3. Semenza JC, Rubin HC, Falter KH, Selanikio JD, Flanders DW, Wilhelm JL: Risk factors for heat-related mortality during the July 1995 heat wave in Chicago. New Eng J Med. 1996, 335: 84-90. 10.1056/NEJM199607113350203.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Semenza JC, McCullough J, Flanders DW, McGeehin MA, Lumpkin JR: Excess hospital admissions during the 1995 heat wave in Chicago. Am J Prev Med. 1999, 16: 269-277. 10.1016/S0749-3797(99)00025-2.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Broder JMA, Tintinalli J: Injuries from the 2002 North Carolina ice storm, and strategies for prevention. Injury. 2005, 36: 21-26. 10.1016/j.injury.2004.08.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Hajat SEK, Kovats S, Menne B, Edwards S, Haines A: The human health consequences of flooding in Europe and the Implications for public health: a review of the literature. Applied Environmental and Science Public Health. 2003, 1: 13-21.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Semenza JC, Menne B: Climate Change and Infectious Diseases in Europe. Lancet ID. 2009, 9: 365-375. 10.1016/S1473-3099(09)70104-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. McCarthy JJCO, Leary NA, Dokken DJ, White KS, (Ed.): Climate change 2001: Imacts, Adaptation, and Vulnerability. 2001, Cambridge Cambridge University Press

  9. Haines AKR, Campbell-Lendrum D, Corvalan C: Climate change and human health: impacts, vulnerability, and mitigation. Lancet. 2006, 367: 2101-2109. 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68933-2.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Ebi KL, Semenza JC: Community-based adaptation to the health impacts of climate change. Am J Prev Med. 2008, 35: 501-507. 10.1016/j.amepre.2008.08.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. McCarthy JOF, Canziani NL, Dokken DJ, White KS, (eds) (Ed.): Climate Change 2001, Impacts, Adaptation, and Vulnerability 2001: Impacts, Adaptation and Vulnerability: Contribution of Working Group II to the Third Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. 2001, Cambridge, UK and New York.: Cambridge University Pres

  12. Kerr RA: Amid worrisome signs of warming, "climate fatigue" sets in. Science. 2009, 326: 926-928. 10.1126/science.326.5955.926.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Hale J, Dillard J, (Eds.): Fear appeals in health promotion campaigns: Too much, too little, or just right?. 1995, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications

  14. Nisbet MC: Communicating climate change: Why frames matter for public engagement. Environment. 2009, 51: 514-518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Campbell-Lendrum DBR: Science, media and public perception: Implications for climate and health policies. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 2010, 88: 242-242. 10.2471/BLT.10.077362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Akerlof KDR, Berry P, Leiserowitz A, Roser-Renouf C, Clarke KL, Rogaeva A, Nisbet MC, Weathers MR, Maibach EW: Public perceptions of climate change as a human health risk: surveys of the United States, Canada and Malta. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2010, 7: 2559-2606. 10.3390/ijerph7062559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Gallup: Americans' Global Warming Concerns Continue to Drop. Book Americans' Global Warming Concerns Continue to Drop. 2010

    Google Scholar 

  18. Leiserowitz AA: Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: The role of affect, imagery and values. Climatic Change. 2006, 77: 45-72. 10.1007/s10584-006-9059-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Semenza JCHD, Wilson DJ, Bontempo BD, Sailor DJ, George LA: Public perception of climate change voluntary mitigation and barriers to behavior change. Am J Prev Med. 2008, 35: 479-487. 10.1016/j.amepre.2008.08.020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Fishbein MAI, (Ed.): Belief, attitude, intention and behaviour. 1975, New York: Addison-Wesley

  21. Bandura A, (Ed.): Social learning theory. 1977, NJ: Prentice-Hall

  22. Rosenstock IM, (Ed.): The health belief model:explaning health behaviour through expectancies. 1900, San Francisco: Jossey-Bass

  23. Brody SDZS, Vedlitz A, Grover H: Examining the relationship between physical vulnerability and public perceptions of global climate change in the United States. Environment and Behavior. 2008, 40: 72-95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Lichtenberg EZR: Adverse health experiences, environmental attitudes, and pesticide usage behavior of farm operators. Risk Analysis. 1999, 19: 283-294.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Skov TCT, Jensen LK, Saugman P, Schmidt K, Theilade P: Modifications of health behaviour in response to air pollution notifications in Copenhagen. Social Science and Medicine. 1991, 33: 621-626. 10.1016/0277-9536(91)90220-7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Biesbroek GRSR, Knaap GM van der: The mitigation-adaptation dichotomy and the role of spatial planning. Habitat International. 2008, 33: 230-237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Steeh CKN, Cannon B, DeWitt J: Are they really as bad as they seem? Nonresponse rates at the end of the twentieth century. Journal of Official Statistics. 2001, 17: 227-247.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Groves RMCM, (Ed.): Nonresponse in household surveys. 1998, New York: Wiley

  29. Link MWKM: The future of random-digit-dial surveys for injury prevention and violence research. Am J Prev Med. 2006, 31: 444-450. 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.07.017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Rosenstock IM, Strecher VJ, Becker MH: Social Learning Theory and the Health Belief Model. Health Education & Behavior. 1988, 15: 175-183. 10.1177/109019818801500203.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. De Stavola BL, Nitsch D, Silva ID, McCormack V, Hardy R, Mann V, Cole TJ, Morton S, Leon DA: Statistical issues in life course epidemiology. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2006, 163: 84-96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. MacKinnon DPLC, Brown Wang WCH, Hoffman JM: The intermediate endpoint effect in logistic and probit regression. Clinical Trials. 2007, 4: 499-513. 10.1177/1740774507083434.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Muthen LK, Muthen BO: Mplus User's Guide. Fourth Edition. 1998, Los Angeles, CA

    Google Scholar 

  34. Stern PCDT, Abel T, Guagnano GA, Kalof L: A Value-Belief-Norm Theory of Support for Social Movements: The Case of Environmentalism. Human Ecology Review. 1999, 6: 81-97.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Stern PC: Towards a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. Jouranl of Social Issues. 2000, 56: 407-424. 10.1111/0022-4537.00175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Lorenzoni IN-CS, Whitmarsh L: Barriers perceived to engagning with climate change among the UK public and their policy implications. Global Environmental Change. 2007, 17: 445-459. 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2007.01.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Attari SZDM, Davidson CI, Bruine de Bruin W: Public perceptions of energy consumption and savings. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010, 107: 16054-16059. 10.1073/pnas.1001509107.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Whitmarsh L: Behavioural responses to climate change: Asymmetry of intentions and impacts. J Environ Psychology. 2009, 29: 13-23. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.05.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Whitmarsh L: Are flood victims more concerned about climate change than other people? the role of direct experience in risk perception and behavioural response. Journal of Risk Research. 2008, 11: 351-374. 10.1080/13669870701552235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Malka AKJ, Langer G: The association of knowledge with concern about global warming: Trusted information sources shape public thinking. Risk Analysis. 2009, 29: 633-647. 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2009.01220.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Kellstedt PMZS, Vedlitz A: Personal efficacy, the information environment, and attitudes toward global warming and climate change in the United States. Risk Analysis. 2008, 28: 113-126. 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01010.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Maibach EWNM, Baldwin P, Akerlof K, Diao G: Reframing climate change as a public health issue: an exploratory study of public reactions. BMC Public Health. 2010, 10: 299-10.1186/1471-2458-10-299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Reiner DMCT, De Figueiredo MA, Herzog HJ, Ansolabehere SD, Itaoka K, Johnsson F, Odenberger M: American exceptionalism? Similarities and differences in national attitudes toward energy policy and global warming. Environ Sci Technol. 2006, 40: 2093-2098. 10.1021/es052010b.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Bord RJFA, O'Connor RE: Public perceptions of global warming: United States and international perspectives. Climate Research. 1998, 11: 75-84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Brechin SR: Comparative public opinion and knowledge on global climatic change and the Kyoto protocol: The US vs the rest of the world?. International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy. 2003, 23: 106-134. 10.1108/01443330310790318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Kempton W: How the public views climate change. Environment. 1997, 39: 12-21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Semenza JCWD, Parra J, Bontempo BD, Hart M, Sailor DJ, George LA: Public perception and behavior change in relationship to hot weather and air pollution. Environ Res. 2008, 107: 401-411. 10.1016/j.envres.2008.03.005.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Maibach EWCA, McBride D, Chuk M, Ebi KL, Balbus J: Climate change and local public health in the United States: Preparedness, programs and perceptions of local public health department directors. PLoS ONE. 2008, 3: e2838-10.1371/journal.pone.0002838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. World Health Organization G: Protecting Health from Climate change: Connecting Science, Policy and People. Book Protecting Health from Climate change: Connecting Science, Policy and People. 2009, City, (Editor ed.^eds.).

    Google Scholar 

  50. Wilkinson PSK, Davies M, Adair H, Armstrong BG, Barrett M, Bruce N, Haines A, Hamilton I, Oreszczyn T, Ridley I, Tonne C, Chalabi Z: Public health benefits of strategies to reduce greenhouse-gas emissions: household energy. Lancet. 2009, 374: 1917-1929. 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61713-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Zahran S, Brody SD, Grover H, Vedlitz A: Climate change vulnerability and policy support. Society and Natural Resources. 2006, 19: 771-789. 10.1080/08941920600835528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Leiserowitz AA: American risk perceptions: Is climate change dangerous?. Risk Analysis. 2005, 25: 1433-1442. 10.1111/j.1540-6261.2005.00690.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Krosnick JAHA, Lowe L, Visser PS: The Origins and consequences of democratic citizens' policy agendas: A study of popular concern about global warming. Climatic Change. 2006, 7-43.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Slimak MWDT: Personal values, beliefs, and ecological risk perception. Risk Analysis. 2006, 26: 1689-1705. 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2006.00832.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Semenza JC: Strategies to intervene on social determinants of infectious diseases. Euro Surveill. 2010, 15: 32-39.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  56. Spence APN: Framing and communicating climate change: The effects of distance and outcome frame manipulations. Global Environmental Change. 2010,

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Amber Johnson, Debi Elliott and the survey team from the Survey Research Laboratory Portland State University for their diligent work on this project. We would like to thank Drs Kristie Ebi, John Balbus, George Luber, Jeremy Hess, Michele Bernardi, Tek-Ang Lim and Jonathan Suk for critical feedback on the paper. This work was supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. 0410103. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jan C Semenza.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

JCS designed the study, developed the questionnaire, supervised the survey, analyzed the data and wrote the paper. GBP conducted the parallel logistic regressions, path analysis and contributed to the writing. LAG contributed to the questionnaire development, survey, and writing of the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Semenza, J.C., Ploubidis, G.B. & George, L.A. Climate change and climate variability: personal motivation for adaptation and mitigation. Environ Health 10, 46 (2011). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1476-069X-10-46

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1476-069X-10-46

Keywords